Thursday, September 10, 2015

Gaydar



I saw this on HuffPost Gay Voices and found it pretty interesting.

Is Gaydar Real?
By Rebecca Adams

Academic studies can be fascinating... and totally confusing. So we decided to strip away all of the scientific jargon and break them down for you. 

The Background

Whether or not "gaydar" -- a supposed intuitive ability to identify gay people -- is real, many people believe it's possible to tell someone's sexual orientation just by looking at them. The problem is, research (and anecdotal evidence) has found that gaydar tends to rely on stereotypical attributes -- like the way someone dresses or how they style their hair -- that don't actually tell you anything about who someone's attracted to. Gaydar, therefore, seems to legitimize these stereotypical myths, something that's been shown to lead to prejudice and oppression. Unlike other forms of stereotypes, however, gaydar has seeped it's way into popular culture, and it's considered relatively harmless and socially acceptable. 

In a new five-part study, researchers from the University of Wisconsin-Madison set out to see if what they refer to as "the gaydar myth" is as "harmless" as some people may think or if it's just a veiled method of perpetuating gay stereotypes. 

The Setup

In the first study, participants looked at pictures of 55 gay men and 50 straight men's faces selected from an online dating site. Each photo was rated for overall quality, from "very poor" to "excellent," by a set of student raters before the study. Then, the researchers randomly paired the photos with a supposed descriptive statement about the person that was either gay-stereotypic ("He likes shopping."), stereotype-neutral ("He likes to read.") or straight-stereotypic ("He likes football."). These weren't actually applicable to the men in the photos, but participants didn't know that. They were then instructed to determine whether or not the man in the photo was gay. For the second study, the researchers repeated the first study, but this time they only chose photos that were rated highest in quality from both the straight and gay men groups of photos. 

Both the first and second studies found that when participants were given stereotypically gay personal statements with photos, they were much more likely to guess that the man in the photo was gay. Meaning: The pictures didn't matter nearly as much as the stereotypes did.

The third study had participants categorize the same gay and straight men's pictures without the accompanying stereotypic statements. The researchers found that people were more likely to assume men in higher quality photos were gay -- they seemingly assumed gay men would take better photos. The fourth study replicated the third with women's photos instead of men's to see if the same was true for lesbians. Participants were unable to gauge sexual orientation simply by looking at a person's face.

Finally, the researchers did their fifth study to determine whether or not gaydar serves as a legitimizing myth for these stereotypes. They gathered 233 undergraduate participants and divided them into three groups: one that would be told that gaydar is stereotyping, one that would be told that gaydar is real and one that would be given no information regarding gaydar. Participants then completed a modified version of the first study, using the same pictures and statements. This time, however, participants could refrain from guessing the person's sexual orientation if they wanted. 

The Findings

In the final study, participants' answers depended on which group they were in. Those in the "gaydar is real" group tended to believe in gaydar more than the other groups, and people in the "gaydar is stereotyping" group believed in it less than the control group. In this final version of the study, it was easy to see that people didn't assign sexual orientation simply because they were forced to choose -- participants had a "no idea" option, yet they chose it "very infrequently," according to the study.

As the researchers put it: "The evidence provided in Study 5 indicates that the folk concept of gaydar serves as a legitimizing myth, promoting stereotyping to infer orientation by giving that stereotyping process the alternate label of 'gaydar.'" Basically, when people slap on a euphemism for stereotyping -- in this case, "gaydar" -- they feel free to judge groups of people by very limited parameters which legitimize societal myths. These findings build on past research about how stereotypes that seem plausible will likely lead to inaccurate assumptions.

The Takeaway

Taken at face value, the concept of gaydar may not seem like such a big deal, but there's one big problem with stereotyping: It often leads to inaccurate conclusions. The researchers put it in terms of the "gay men like shopping" trope. If people assume gay men like shopping, that doesn't mean that all men who like shopping are gay (or that all gay men like shopping). Not to mention, if gay men make up 1.8 percent of the male population in America, even if they're ten times more likely to enjoy shopping, men who like shopping are still more likely to be straight -- there are simply more men who identify as straight out there. 

Perhaps the researchers put it best: "Whether people fit or violate their group's stereotypes is immaterial to their value -- we would hope that, rather than being judged or pressured based on the existence of a stereotype, people can be treated as individuals and judged on their own merit." Amen.

The Closet Professor’s Conclusion

It seems to me that the study has two major flaws.  First, it assumes that gaydar is purely visual and can be determined by a picture of a face.  When my “gaydar” goes off, it's more than just a picture of a face.  It has to do with how he moves, how he talks, and basically, how he carries himself.  The most sure fire way is to watch his eyes.  If a hot guy walks by and his eyes follow, then he is probably gay, but if a hot girl walks by and his eyes follow her, then he is probably straight.  You have to watch the eyes though, because head movements can be misleading, especially for someone in the closet. 

Second, the study assumes that gaydar is something that heterosexual men possess.  While I do think that some women possess gaydar, most straight men do not.  Heterosexual men often use all kinds of bad stereotypes to identify gay men; however, gay men and some women use more subtle stereotypes to identify gay men. I do not dispute that a large part of gaydar is stereotyping, but I think gay men tend to be more careful with stereotyping and are more intuitive. Many gay men were stereotyped before they came out, so they aren't as quick to judge others unfairly.  That being said, I will postulate that wishful thinking does occasionally interferes with gaydar.

Finally, I think gaydar is possibly an evolutionary characteristic.  Gay men have always existed, but we had to find one another.  Historically, if a gay man hit on the wrong man, i.e. a straight man, then he might not survive the attempt.  Therefore, I think along with the genetic code that makes us gay, we also have the ability to find one another.  Then again, gaydar could be a complete myth built on stereotypes, but I think it is very real, some people just have better gaydar than others.  I tend to think mine is pretty good.

6 comments:

Susan said...

Another excellent post, Joe. Your conclusions are what makes sense to me. I would never presume to know anyone's sexual orientation by looking at a picture of their face. And you are spot on concerning straight men in this area. :) Glad to hear your gaydar is in good shape.

Unknown said...

I agree, if there is such a thing as gaydar, it is a lot more than just looking at a picture. Voice and mannerisms have a lot to do with it as well.

I for one believe that there is some truth to most stereotypes. Also, a certain "campy" way gay males tend to behave when congregating together. But there are a lot of exceptions to stereotypes as well, thus it is not fair to paint everyone with the same brush.

Michael Dodd said...
This comment has been removed by the author.
Michael Dodd said...

[I deleted the first version of this because I noticed too late a typo that confused the meaning. Any remaining confusion is my own fault!]

"Something about the eyes" is what I always notice. Years ago, a behavioral psychologist told me that the only behavioral thing that people in his field had found that indicated a person was homosexual was that we tend to keep eye contact a nanosecond longer -- but for us, that is enough. So it is not how a person looks/appears, but how he looks/makes contact.

I do think this shows up, however, in certain photographs of people who open their eyes in such a way that the click happens. The first time I saw a photo of Ricky Martin in 1999 after the release of "La vida loca" and knowing absolutely nothing about who he was at the time, I immediately commented to a friend, "That man is gay."

Whatever.

Amanda said...

I've met gay men who said that they're gaydar was undoubtedly accurate and on the other side some who said theirs was broken. Go figure. It's good that yours is in working order. :)

Joe said...

I'm trying out this new app called BlogTouch which is supposed to make responding to comments easier, and I think it will, but one of the features of its "artificial intelligence" is that it does comment sentiment. It "tells" me that this post had 2 neutral comments (Amanda's and Michael's deleted comment), two positive (Susan's and Andrew's), and one negative (Michael's). I'd have said that all were positive, at least that's how I took them. If I understood programming, is love to see how their algorithms work.

Thanks everyone for your comments.